Research Article / Open Access
DOI: 10.31488/bjcr.149
The Reliability and Validity of the Japanese Version of Revised Illness Perception Questionnaires for Healthy People (IPQ-RH-J)
Hiroko Terui-Kohbata*1,2, Masami Ikeda3,Kei Yura4,5
Life Science and Bioethics Research Center, Tokyo Medical and Dental University, Tokyo, Japan
Department of Medical Genetics, Tokyo Medical and Dental University, Tokyo, Japan
Faculty of Education and Humanities, Department of Psychology, Jumonji University, Saitama, Japan
Graduate School of Humanities and Sciences, Ochanomizu University, Tokyo, Japan
School of Advanced Science and Engineering, Waseda University, Tokyo, Japan
*Corresponding author: Hiroko Terui-Kohbata, Ph.D., C.G.C, Life Science and Bioethics Research Center, Tokyo Medicaland Dental University, 1-5-45 Yushima, Bunkyo, Tokyo 113-8510, Japan,Tel: 81-3-5803-4085; Fax:81-3-5803-4725
Abstract
We verified the reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Revised Illness Perception Questionnaire for Healthy people (IPQ-RH-J) because Japanese scales related illness perceptions for healthy peopledoes not exist. The illness perceptions of two diseases with different characteristic, namely breast cancer (BC) and diabetes (DM), were assessed in 159 females and 109 males in Japan. Using the negative affectivity subscales of the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (NA-PANAS), the discriminant validity was assessed. IPQ-RH-J achieved good scores on all the tests including the construct validity (Comparative Fit of Index (CFI): 0.93 in BC, 0.88 in DM), the discriminant validity (Pearson’s correlation (r) < 0.2 in BC and DM) and the test-retest reliability (Cronbach’s α: 0.59 to 0.92 in BC, 0.57 to 0.90 in DM).Comparison between the illness perception for breast cancer and that for diabetes revealed the differences in the duration of symptoms, the severity of consequences, feelings of self-control, the emotional representations, and the recognition of the genetic factors.We found that IPQ-RH is universally effective across different cultures and can beused for diseases with gender differences.
Keywords: illness perception, IPQ, healthy people, breast cancer
Introduction
Leventhal’s common-sense model of the illness representations can be used to understand person’s illness cognition that is beliefs or perception of illness[1]. The illness representationsexhibit dynamic change and has been developed together with a range of resources, including direct experience with disease and medical care; indirect experience through family, friends, and media; and cultural thinking. The theory comprisesfive core perspectives: 1)identity of the threat, the label representing the idea of the disease based on their experiences and symptoms; 2) cause, the person’s ideas about the perceived cause of the illness, namely infection, genetic, stresses, or other sources; 3) timeline, the individual’s perceived clinical course and duration of the illness; 4) consequences, the perceived impact of the illness including both physical and psychological effects; and 5) control, the controllability of prevention and treatment.
To understand illness cognition, a scale using the illness representations model (the Illness Perception Questionnaire: IPQ) has been developed [2]. Subsequently, a revised version of the illness cognition scale (the Revised IPQ: IPQ-R) was prepared[3]. These scales determine how specific groups and individuals, such as patients and those at risk for certain diseases, perceive their diseases. However, there have been few studies that describe how healthy people think of health and diseaseand how their thinking is linked to healthy activities. In response to this set of factors, a scale of illness cognition for healthy people (IPQ-R for healthy people: IPQ-RH) was developed[4] to understand illness cognition in society in general.
Many studies have found andsupport that an array of complex factors related to disease, such as experience, knowledge, and culture, are major deciding factors in the representation of illness (e.g.,[5]).When the cause of a disease is genetic, it is often conceptualized as something abstract, uncontrollable, and inexplicable. Such an interpretation is said to be related to “genetic exceptionalism”, expressed as a social perspective on genetic information [6]. In the setting of genetic counseling dealing with hereditary diseases, individual differences in the elements based on the illness representation model lead to different recognitions of risk, cost, and benefit with respect to decision making in medical care [7]. Belief and cognition in relation to hereditary diseases are influential factors on genetic risk perception (e.g., [5]).Therefore, it is important for healthcare professionals in the setting of genetic counseling to understand how a client perceives and believes their illness.To understand clients’ illness cognition and prepare appropriate support in genetic counseling, formation and transitionof illness cognition must be understood correctly. To that end, it is important to understand the illness cognition of healthy people in general society that can affect the client's personal experience, knowledge, and cultural background.
In this study, we prepared the Japanese version of IPQ-RH (IPQ-RH-J), which was developed as a scale of illness cognition for healthy people in general Japanese society. The translation and cultural adaptation process was based on the report of Wildet al.[8]. We obtained permission to prepare IPQ-RH-J from the author of IPQ-RH and finalized IPQ-RH-J through the following four steps;forward translation, back translation, debriefing, and author’s confirmation.The present survey verified the reliability and validity of IPQ-RH-J.
Methods
Subjects
We recruited 268 Japanese men and women (aged 20 to 80 years old). The ideal sample size was calculated using the COSMIN (Consensus-Based Standards for the Selection of Health Measurement Instruments) checklist and literaturesonthe development of IPQ related scales[2-4,9]. Because IPQ-RHwas designed for healthy people, subjects with a past/current history of breast cancer or diabetes were asked not to answer questions related to these diseases.
Questionnaire survey
Informed consent was obtained before answering the questionnaire. To verify reliability and validity, we used three scales: IPQ-RH-J (26 items), the negative affectivity of the Japanese version of Positive and Negative Affect Scale (NA-PANAS) (8 items)[10], and the causal attribution items (16 items, hereinafter described cause). Responses were taken on a five-point Likert scale (from 1 = Strongly disagree to 5 = Strongly agree)for IPQ-RH-J,a six-point Likert scale (from 1 = Not at all to 6 = Extremely) was used for NA-PANAS, and for the causeattribution, we had subjects choose “yes” or “no,” that is, whichever they felt was most accurate. Information on subjects’ age, sex, and level of education were also gathered.
The survey was conducted on those with consent for this research from December 2018 to February 2019 by web or post mail. We asked the test–retest group to repeat the questionnaire after a three-week interval. The anonymized ID was used so that two individual responses could be linked in the test–retest group and the other subjects were anonymous.
Statistical analyses
To verify the validity of IPQ-RH-J, we performed confirmatory factor analysis and calculated the fit of the model (GFI: Goodness of Fit Index, AGFI: Adjusted Goodness of Fit Index, CFI: Comparative Fit of Index, and RMSEA: Root Mean Square Error of Approximation) for seven subscales of IPQ-RH-J such as "timeline acute/chronic", "timeline cyclical", "consequences", "personal control", "the treatment control", "illness coherence" and "emotional representations" in the IPQ-RH.GFI and AGFAI show how much the variance-covariance matrix of the estimated model can reproduce the variance-covariance matrix of the actual observation data. CFI is an index of comparative fitness based on how much the model fit improved when compared to the independent model. RMSEA is an index of frugality correction showing the degree of deviation per degree of freedom. In order to verify discriminant validity, we obtained correlation coefficients for the total NA-PANASscores and the subscale scores. We used the test–retest reliability method. We obtained a total score of the subscale from two answers and calculated Cronbach’s alpha and Pearson correlation coefficients. The level of statistical significance was set to p< 0.05. For statistical analysis, we used SPSS Statistics 26.0.
Results
Respondents’ characteristics
The demographicsof respondents isshownin Table 1. Among the group of 268 respondents, 109 were men and 159 were women, with an average age of 46.4 years old (ranging from 21 to 79 years old).
Table 1. The demographic features of respondents
All respondents | ||
Sex | N | (%) |
Male | 109 | 40.7 |
Female | 159 | 59.3 |
Total | 268 | |
Age | years old | |
Average | 46.4 | (21-79) |
Final education | N | (%) |
Junior high school | 8 | 3.0 |
Senior high school | 64 | 23.9 |
College | 72 | 26.9 |
Undergraduate school | 76 | 28.3 |
Master 's degree | 29 | 10.8 |
Doctor's degree | 19 | 7.1 |
Test-retest group | ||
Sex | N | (%) |
Male | 20 | 32.8 |
Female | 41 | 67.2 |
Total | 61 | |
Age | years old | |
Average | 44.5 | (22-71) |
Verification of Construct Validity
To calculate the fit of the model, we used SPSS Statistics 26.0. After preparing the path diagram using all 26 items, we added five paths to improve the fit. By addingpaths between related items for each subscale, we maximized the fit for both breast cancer and diabetes. The added paths were “this illness will last a short time”to“this illness will pass quickly” (I) and “I expect this illness to last for the rest of one’s life”to“this illness is likely to be permanent rather than temporary” (II) in the subscale of the timeline acute/chronic, “the symptom comes and goes in cycles” to “this illness goes through cycles in which gets better or worse” (III) in the timeline cyclical, “this illness is a mystery to me”to“I don’t understand this illness” (IV) in the illness coherence, and “this illness makes me afraid”to“thinking about having this illness makes me feel anxious”(V) in the emotional representations. Paths I and II wereset between the items related to the short or very long duration. Path III was set between the items related to the periodicity of symptoms. Path IV was set between the items related to inability to understand logically and emotionally.Path V was set between the items of negative feelings for the illness. The resulting fit of the model for breast cancer was measured as follows: GFI= 0.878, AGFI= 0.843, CFI= 0.927, and RMSEA= 0.056; for diabetes, these values were GFI= 0.866, AGFI= 0.827, CFI= 0.884, and RMSEA= 0.064.
Verification of Discriminant Validity
The verification results of discriminant validity are shown in Table 2. The correlation coefficients betweenNA-PANAS and the seven subscales for IPQ-RH-Jwere less than 0.2 for all items under diabetes and breast cancer, not indicating any correlation. However, there were correlations for the seven subscales of IPQ-RH-J, thus confirming its validity.
Table 2. Correlation between subscales of IPQ-RH-J
Scales | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 |
Breast Cancer (n=268) | ||||||||||
1. Timeline acute/chronic | ― | |||||||||
2. Timeline cyclical | 0.299*** | ― | ||||||||
3. Consequences | 0.405*** | 0.285*** | ― | |||||||
4. Personal control | 0.036 | 0.270*** | 0.108 | ― | ||||||
5. Treatment control | 0.055 | 0.139* | 0.241*** | 0.164*** | ― | |||||
6. Illness coherence | 0.177** | 0.346*** | 0.335*** | 0.278*** | 0.03 | ― | ||||
7. Emotional representations | 0.213*** | 0.301*** | 0.382*** | 0.182** | 0.139* | 0.555*** | ― | |||
8. Psychological attributions | -0.093 | -0.078 | -0.116 | -0.113 | 0.02 | -0.028 | -0.089 | ― | ||
9. General risk factors | -0.118 | -0.076 | -0.041 | -0.084 | 0.042 | -0.022 | -0.120* | 0.594*** | ― | |
10. NA-PANAS | 0.076 | 0.06 | 0.039 | -0.067 | -0.109 | 0.072 | 0.147* | -0.027 | -0.091 | ― |
Diabetes (n=267) | ||||||||||
1. Timeline acute/chronic | ― | |||||||||
2. Timeline cyclical | 0.087 | ― | ||||||||
3. Consequences | 0.495*** | 0.280*** | ― | |||||||
4. Personal control | 0.202** | 0.056 | 0.323*** | ― | ||||||
5. Treatment control | 0.142* | 0.041 | 0.114 | 0.317*** | ― | |||||
6. Illness coherence | -0.054 | 0.326*** | 0.267*** | 0.054 | -0.009 | ― | ||||
7. Emotional representations | 0.174** | 0.202** | 0.362*** | 0.101 | 0.044 | 0.479*** | ― | |||
8. Psychological attributions | -0.026 | -0.045 | -0.123* | -0.123* | -0.008 | -0.052 | -0.213*** | ― | ||
9. General risk factors | -0.116 | -0.071 | -0.132* | -0.014 | 0.045 | -0.107 | -0.161** | 0.500*** | ― | |
10. NA-PANAS | 0.031 | 0.058 | -0.002 | -0.12 | -0.096 | 0.107 | 0.177** | -0.132* | -0.150* | ― |
*** p<.001, **p<.01, *p<.05
Verification of the Test–Retest Reliabilities
Table 3 shows the reliability verification result with the test–retest reliability method. Cronbach’s alpha for the seven subscales was 0.590 to 0.919 for breast cancer and 0.573 to 0.898 for diabetes, excluding 0.36 for the cyclical timeline (first time). The Pearson correlation coefficient was 0.339 to 0.794 for breast cancer and 0.387 to 0.760 for diabetes. For all subscales, the result was significant, with p< 0.001.
Table 3. Internal consistency and test-retest reliability of IPQ-RH-J dimension
Breast cancer (n=61) | Diabetes (n=60) | |||||
Cronbach's α | Pearson's correlation | Cronbach's α | Pearson's correlation | |||
Test | Re-test | Test | Re-test | |||
Timeline acute/chronic | 0.670 | 0.771 | 0.570*** | 0.776 | 0.870 | 0.387** |
Timeline cyclical | 0.590 | 0.697 | 0.571*** | 0.036 | 0.573 | 0.430** |
Consequences | 0.629 | 0.644 | 0.755*** | 0.713 | 0.650 | 0.760*** |
Personal control | 0.738 | 0.814 | 0.493*** | 0.773 | 0.781 | 0.638*** |
Treatment control | 0.694 | 0.640 | 0.339** | 0.792 | 0.663 | 0.663*** |
Illness coherence | 0.682 | 0.779 | 0.794*** | 0.604 | 0.819 | 0.687*** |
Emotional representations | 0.919 | 0.918 | 0.771*** | 0.866 | 0.898 | 0.750*** |
*** p<.001, **p<.01
Cognition Related to the Cause of illness
For 16 items on the cause of disease, the subjects answered “yes” or “no” in relation to diabetes and breast cancer (Table 4). Following the previous studies, we divided psychological attributions and general risk factors and calculated the alpha coefficient, finding α= 0.707 and α= 0.556 for diabetes and α= 0.755 and α= 0.711 for breast cancer. The results for breast cancer and diabetes showed no significant difference among the diseases for each cause of psychological attribution, but there was a significant difference in causes “germ or virus”, “immunity”, “alcohol”, “pollution”, “chance or bad luck”, “own behavior”, and “poor diet”for general risk factors.
Table 4. . Perceptions of causal attributions
Breast cancer (n=268) | Diabetes (n=267) | Chi-squared test | |||||||||
Yes | No | Yes | No | ||||||||
Psychological attributions | (α=.755) | (α=.707) | |||||||||
Cause3 | Overwork | 92 | 34.3% | 176 | 65.7% | 102 | 38.2% | 165 | 61.8% | n.s. | |
Cause4 | Personality | 91 | 34.0% | 177 | 66.0% | 109 | 40.8% | 158 | 59.2% | n.s. | |
Cause6 | Emotional state | 92 | 34.3% | 176 | 65.7% | 84 | 31.5% | 183 | 68.5% | n.s. | |
Cause7 | Mental attitude | 79 | 29.5% | 189 | 70.5% | 73 | 27.3% | 194 | 72.7% | n.s. | |
Cause8 | Family problems | 90 | 33.6% | 178 | 66.4% | 110 | 41.2% | 157 | 58.8% | n.s. | |
Cause10 | Stress or worry | 144 | 53.7% | 124 | 46.3% | 139 | 52.1% | 128 | 47.9% | n.s. | |
General risk factors | (α=.711) | (α=.556) | |||||||||
Cause1 | Heredity | 209 | 78.0% | 59 | 22.0% | 199 | 74.5% | 68 | 25.5% | n.s. | |
Cause2 | Germ or virus | 51 | 19.0% | 217 | 81.0% | 20 | 7.5% | 247 | 92.5% | ** | |
Cause5 | Immunity | 165 | 61.6% | 103 | 38.4% | 91 | 34.1% | 176 | 65.9% | *** | |
Cause9 | Aging | 143 | 53.4% | 125 | 46.6% | 156 | 58.4% | 111 | 41.6% | n.s. | |
Cause11 | Alcohol | 90 | 33.6% | 178 | 66.4% | 213 | 79.8% | 54 | 20.2% | *** | |
Cause12 | Smoking | 127 | 47.4% | 141 | 52.6% | 126 | 47.2% | 141 | 52.8% | n.s. | |
Cause13 | Accident or injury | 21 | 7.8% | 247 | 92.2% | 20 | 7.5% | 247 | 92.5% | n.s. | |
Cause14 | Pollution | 62 | 23.1% | 206 | 76.9% | 33 | 12.4% | 234 | 87.6% | ** | |
Cause15 | Poor medical care | 78 | 29.1% | 190 | 70.9% | 71 | 26.6% | 196 | 73.4% | n.s. | |
Cause16 | Chance or bad luck | 149 | 55.6% | 119 | 44.4% | 69 | 25.8% | 198 | 74.2% | *** | |
Cause17 | Own behavior | 70 | 26.1% | 198 | 73.9% | 196 | 73.4% | 71 | 26.6% | *** | |
Cause18 | Poor diet | 139 | 51.9% | 129 | 48.1% | 249 | 93.3% | 18 | 6.7% | *** |
*** p<.001, **p<.01, *p<.05
Known group validity
Known-groups validity was assessed by comparing the subscale score among diabetes and breast cancer.The subscale scoresof IPQ-RH-J and the scores of the two attributional factors are shown in Table 5. Significant differences were detected in subscales other than the timeline cyclical and the general risk factors.
Table 5. Average of subscale scores of IPQ-RH-J and causal attributions
Average of subscale score | T-test | |||||
Breast cancer | Diabetes | P value | ||||
(n=267) | (n=268) | |||||
IPQ-RH-J | ||||||
Timeline acute/chronic | 18.3 | 21.0 | *** | |||
Timeline cyclical | 9.0 | 9.0 | n.s. | |||
Consequences | 15.1 | 14.5 | ** | |||
Personal control | 8.3 | 10.5 | *** | |||
Treatment control | 10.5 | 10.9 | * | |||
Illness coherence | 9.7 | 8.8 | *** | |||
Emotional representations | 17.2 | 15.4 | *** | |||
Causal attributions | ||||||
Psychological attributions | 3.8 | 3.7 | n.s. | |||
General risk factors | 7.1 | 6.6 | *** | |||
*** p<.001, **p<.01, *p<.05 |
Discussion
In this research, we verified the reliability and validity ofIPQ-RH-Jusing two diseases with different characteristics, breast cancer and diabetes. Sincesimilar results were obtained for both diseases, it was found that the present scale was applicable regardless of the characteristics of the disease. The occurrence risk of breast cancer is significantly higher in women than men. In this survey, in which a similar number of men and womenparticipated, the scale reliability and validity was verified regardless of disease characteristics related to sex. Therefore, this scale can be used for disease with gender differences. In future, an additional examination with a different group of diseases is necessary for the use of IPQ-RH-J as a general scale for illness cognition.
Petraket al.[9] examined the fit of IPQ-RH in Croatian and Lebanese women in relation to breast cancer and cervical cancer and showeda fit (CFI) of 0.930 to 0.969 (present research: 0.884 to 0.927) and a Cronbach’s alpha (α) of 0.66 to 0.82 (present research: 0.57 to 0.92). The Pearson correlation coefficient (r) in the test–retest reliability method was 0.40 to 0.93 (present research: 0.34 to 0.77). Petrak’s datawere consistently high values. In the original paper on IPQ-RH[4], AIDS, skin cancer, and tuberculosis were examined, with results of α= 0.60 to 0.82 andr= 0.31 to 0.78. Our resultsarecomparable to these studies. Some of our values rare lower than those found in the data of Petraket al.[9].This may be because the diseases that Petraket al.examined were only cancers that shared characteristics, and all their subjects were women. In addition, there may have been relevant cultural and linguistic differences.
Previously, studies have been conducted of the illness cognition of cancer and diabetes based on illness representations [11,12]. Cancer is usually understood as a serious, threatening disease that can lead to death. For its part, diabetes is considered to be an age-related disease and is not closely linked to death. Although environmental and lifestyle triggers are related to cancer, in addition to genetic risk, diabetes features more factors than cancer does for its occurrence, and it is believed that lifestyle and behavior can reduce risk and control symptoms. Thepresent research has also showndifferences in disease characteristics for duration, sense of self-control, impact on emotions, and causes of disease for breast cancer and diabetes in Japan. Illness cognition may differ according to medical system, cultural belief, sex, social status, country, area, and social background. Usage ofIPQ-RH can allow the comparison and understanding of illness cognition according to subject attributes. This will help implement promotion and support measures for healthy behavior based on illness cognition.
One of the major findings of this study is that IPQ-RH has been found to be somewhat universally effective across cultures. On the other hand, as mentioned in the Introduction, it is known that there is "genetic exceptionalism" on genetic information or cause attributions on diseases [6]. This knowledge leads us to ask whether IPQ-RH-J is effective in the setting of genetic counseling. In Japan, discrimination and prejudice against genetic diseases have been a topic of concern[13], and investigationsofyoung healthy people indicated that many subjects had a negative view against “heredity” [14,15].In future, it will be necessary to determine whether IPQ-RH-J is effective for the understanding of the illness perception of the genetic diseases.
The illness representations change with experience [16], and hence illness perception is expected to change as the results of the provision of information through genetic counseling. Therefore, understanding the social illness perception for genetic diseases can help the medical staffs appropriately comprehend and evaluate the developments and changes of the illness perception of the clients.Previous indexes used to evaluate illness cognitionhave been limited to the Japanese version of IPQ-Rfor patients [17]. The potential significance of IPQ-RH-J as an index that objectively evaluates illness cognition of healthy people in general societyis substantial. The next challenge is the application of the present scale to congenital abnormality and hereditary disease.
Acknowledgements
We appreciate Ms. Sayako Takahashi and Ms. Miho Aoki for the development of IPQ-RH-J and Dr. Shiho Takeuchi for recruiting participants of the test-retest group.
Funding
This research was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number JP17K17689.
Authors' Contributions
H. T-K. contributed to the design and implementation of the research, to the analysis of the results and to the writing of the manuscript. M.I. contributed to the analysis of the results and to the writing of the manuscript. K.Y. contributed to implementation of the research and to the writing of the manuscript.
Ethics Approval and Consent to Participate
This study was approved by the Medical Research Ethics Committee of Tokyo Medical and Dental University (No. M2018-200). Informed consent was obtained from all respondents before answering the questionnaire.
Declaration of Interest Statement
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
References
Leventhal H, Meyer D, Nerenz D. The commonsense representation of illness danger. Rachman S, editor. Vol. II, Contributions to medical psychology. Pergamon Press,New York, 1980; pp7–30.
Weinman J, Petrie KJ, Moss-Morris R, et al. The illness perception questionnaire: A new method for assessing the cognitive representation of illness. Psychol Heal.1996; 11(3):431–45.
Moss-Morris R, Weinman J, Petrie K, et al. The Revised Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ-R). Psychol Health.2002; 17(1):1–16.
Figueiras MJ, Alves NC. Lay perceptions of serious illnesses: An adapted version of the Revised Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ-R) for healthy people. Psychol Heal.2007; 22(2):143–58.
Sivell S, Elwyn G, Gaff CL, et al.How risk is perceived, constructed and interpreted by clients in clinical genetics, and the effects on decision making: systematic review. J Genet Coun. 2008; 17(1):30–63.
Murray TH. Genetic exceptionalism and “Future Diaries”: Is genetic information different from other medical information? In Genetic secrets: Protecting privacy and confidentiality in the genetic era. In: MA Rothstein, editor. Genetic secrets: Protecting privacy and confidentiality in the genetic era. New Haven: Yale University Press,1997; pp 60–73.
Shiloh S. Illness representations, self-regulation, and genetic counseling: A theoretical review. J Genet Couns.2006; 15(5): 325-37.
Wild D, Grove A, Martin M, et al.Principles of Good Practice for the Translation and Cultural Adaptation Process for Patient-Reported Outcomes (PRO) Measures: report of the ISPOR task force for translating adaptation. Value Heal2005; 8(2):94–104.
Petrak A, Sherman KA, Fitness J. Validation of the Croatian and Lebanese Revised Illness Perception Questionnaires for Healthy People (IPQ-RH). Eur J Cancer Care.2015; 24(3):355–66.
Sato M, Yasuda A. Development of the Japanese version of Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) scales. Japan Soc Personal Psychol.2001; 9(2):138–9.
Walter FM, Emery J. Perceptions of family history across common diseases: a qualitative study in primary care. FamPract.2006; 23(4):472–80.
Wang C, O’Neill SM, Rothrock N,et al.Comparison of risk perceptions and beliefs across common chronic diseases. Prev Med.2009; 48(2):197–202.
Shirai Y. Idenshishindan o megururinritekimondai. Byoin (Hospital) 2001; 60(12): 1027–30.
Matsumoto T, Morifuji K, Sasaki N, et al.Image of Genetics. Nagasaki daigakuigakubuhokengakkakiyo (Bulletin Nagasaki Univ Sch Heal Sci) 2004; 17(2):17–20.
Terui-Kohbata H, Egawa M, Yura K, et al.Knowledge and attitude of hereditary breast cancer among Japanese university female students. J Hum Genet 2020; 65: 591–599.
Leventhal H, Diefenbach M. The Active Side of Illness Cognition. In: Skelton JA, Croyle RT, editors. Mental Representation in Health and Illness. New York: Springer Verlag.1991; pp247–72.
Katayama F, Kodama M, Osada H. Development of the Japasese version of the Illness Perception Questionnaire: Reliability and validity with hemodialysis data. Japanese J Heal Psychol.2009; 22(2):28–39.
Received: March20, 2020;
Accepted: April28, 2020;
Published: April30, 2020.
To cite this article : Kohbata TH, Ikeda M, Yura K.The reliability and validity of the Japanese version of Revised Illness Perception Questionnaires for Healthy people (IPQ-RH-J). British Journal of Cancer Research. 2020;3:2.
©Kohbata TH, et al.2020.